General Inquiries
Martin Leblanc
contact@whatistruth.life
Brand Inquiries
Dale Campbell
press@whatistruth.life
Operation time
6 am to 8 pm BST
London, UK
Back

What drives overtraining? Being pushed to the physical limits of exercise – Literature review

Introduction

Regular moderate-intensity exercise has been extensively promoted by health and lifestyle professionals for years, and widely used in prevention strategies to keep physical diseases and mental distress away (Gleeson et al., 2011). Most people who regularly exercise have a more positive body image and a better general life satisfaction than non-exercisers (Hausenblas & Fallon, 2006).

However, just like anything else, exercise as well can become addictive and damaging when the fitness goals meet other factors that together overwrite the bodily feedback of how much is too much. The resulting excessive exercise is characterised by a compelling feeling to engage in it, guilt if postponed, and maintenance of it despite being aware of the potential illness it can cause (Seigel & Hetta, 2001). Relatively little is known about the causes of individual vulnerability to excessive exercise, but there are certain theories under the radar of recent research efforts.

Athletic pressure

Excessive exercising has been studied for years within the athletic environment due to its impact on performance. It is understood that improving an athlete’s performance requires completing high-intensity and volume exercises, followed by recovery periods sessions (Rocha et al., 2019). The performance pressure of an elite athlete can, however, disrupt the delicate balance between high-load exercise sessions and adequate recovery periods. The resulting overtraining has serious detrimental effects (known as overtraining syndrome) showing way beyond performance decline. It often causes depression, insomnia, musculoskeletal trauma (Eichner, 1995) and pathological conditions in the hypothalamus, liver, and heart (Rocha et al., 2019).

Recognising the problem before more serious symptoms appear is the responsibility of the trainer, and requires noticing the early symptoms, such as changes in mood, changes in training habit, uncharacteristic behaviour and performance decline. Symptoms, however, are often disregarded and trainers and athletes attempt to compensate for poor performance with more excessive training which leads to a vicious cycle (Schorb et al, 2021). Creating an environment which promotes conscious training and self-awareness to avoid overtraining is amongst the main prevention methods but is often undermined by the widespread beliefs among coaches and athletes that ‘pain makes you stronger’ and ‘you must push through your limits’. Along with the one-size-fits-all training plans that fails to incorporate the individual needs of the athletes, the athletic setup itself becomes the most common factor in developing exercise and eating disorders.

Sociocultural pressure

In the context of hobby and semi-professional athletes, several sociocultural trends contribute to exercise dependency even without the pressure of professional performance. Widespread “healthism” movements, claiming to promote health consciousness, create non-exerciser stereotypes by endorsing the idea that regular exercise is a moral obligation and that those who do not exercise are weak or deviant (Crawford, 1980). Globally streamed reality shows often normalise extreme fitness ideals, reducing health to body fitness and supporting a particular health consciousness that is more of a “fitnessism” than actual health. They promote a very fit body as a sign of self-discipline, self-control, and willpower.

However, fitnessism seems to mainly encourage “aesthetic labour” and support commercial interests to exploit body dissatisfaction (Eriksson, 2022). Both of these ongoing trends—healthism and fitnessism—promote a distorted image of health and idolise the neglection of bodily symptoms, advancing self-abuse in the disguise of self-discipline. 

The sociocultural pressures of healthism and fitnessism bring about harmful exercise and eating patterns in both genders (Halliwell & Harvey, 2006) but a higher occurrence of compulsive exercise was found in case of men (White & Halliwell, 2010) due to the additional pressure derived from the cultural stereotype of a “large man”.

Psychoneurological mechanism

Neuroscientific research on compulsive behaviour found that the brain region, called the ventral striatum, is a central regulator in the dopamine-rewarding system linked to addiction and is also involved in people’s ability to socially connect (Inagaki, 2020). Findings suggests that social connection is the main supplier of the reward system (Lieberman, 2014) and without the ability to connect socially, the neurochemical balance has to be recovered in other ways, resulting in drug-seeking and drug-taking behaviours (Heiling et al., 2016) or other compulsive behaviour. In a socially deprived state, almost anything can become addictive, even seemingly healthy activities such as exercising (Mate, 2010).

The lack of social connections, known as loneliness, is characterised by not having enough reciprocal and authentic human bonds, and is independent of the number of connections one has (Hotard et al., 1989). Loneliness is commonly discussed as the opposite end of the belongingness scale, considering lonely people as having an unmet need to belong (Mellor et al., 2008).

Rook’s (1978) data suggested that contrary to the popular belief that social support creates impact via delivering practical help, that aspect is in fact secondary (except in extreme circumstances in which major assistance is needed), whereas the relationship aspect of the social support carries the main impact. They help via making the person feeling listened to and integrated. Even one companion that enables intimate disclosure—a ‘you got a friend in me’ experience—can keep someone away from the most severe consequence of isolation (Baumeister & Leary, 1995) and compulsive behaviours like exercise dependence, overeating, drug seeking etc. While the lack of social connectedness leaves the reward system deprived, increases the brain’s hypersensitivity to the reward process and predisposes people to addiction. Loneliness predicts well-documented clinical risk factors (Pantell et al., 2013) and has been recognised as a major risk factor for morbidity and mortality (Cacioppo et al., 2014).

Discussion

Building social connections is not without risk though (Ignatius & Kokkonen, 2007); they can be exploitative and traumatising, as modern psychology knows for a long time that most unusual behaviour is an understandable response to damaging relationships (Laing, 1960). That makes seeking for, or investing in, human connections a risky activity. The process requires gradually opening up to someone, sharing more and more personal details and responding to others’ disclosure with similar openness (Altman & Taylor, 1973).

It’s always a bit of a gamble where losing is particularly painful. This explains why it is often replaced by reward-related activities (e.g., feeding, exercise, hookups, substance use, collecting connections etc.) (Lewis et al., 2021). In order for us to safely seek for connections to feel belonging and avoid exercise dependence, we need to develop effective social skills to recognise others’ clues, and adaptive coping strategies to handle when our clues don’t lend well. Both require individual strategies, ranging from reading blogs like this, to finding supportive mental health professionals.


References

Ackard, D. M., Brehm, B. J., & Steffen, J. J. (2002). Exercise and eating disorders in college-aged women: Profiling excessive exercisers. Eating Disorders, 10(1), 31-47.

Adams, J., & Kirkby, R. J. (2002). Excessive exercise as an addiction: A review. Addiction Research & Theory, 10(5), 415-437.

Altman, I. and D. A. Taylor: 1973, Social Penetration: The Development of Interpersonal Relationships (Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York).

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497–529.

Cacioppo, S., Capitanio, J. P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2014). Toward a neurology of loneliness. Psychological bulletin, 140(6), 1464.

Colman, A. (2001). Drugs.(Youth Monitor: a national roundup of recent press reports on youth issues). Youth Studies Australia, 20(4), 4-5.

Crawford, R. (1980). Healthism and the medicalization of everyday life. International journal of health services, 10(3), 365-388.

da Rocha, A. L., Pinto, A. P., Kohama, E. B., Pauli, J. R., de Moura, L. P., Cintra, D. E., … & da Silva, A. S. (2019). The proinflammatory effects of chronic excessive exercise. Cytokine, 119, 57-61.

Davis, C., Katzman, D. K., & Kirsh, C. (1999). Compulsive physical activity in adolescents with anorexia nervosa: a psychobehavioral spiral of pathology. The Journal of nervous and mental disease, 187(6), 336-342.

Eichner, E. R. (1995). Overtraining: consequences and prevention. Journal of Sports Sciences, 13(S1), S41-S48.

Gleeson, M., Bishop, N. C., Stensel, D. J., Lindley, M. R., Mastana, S. S., & Nimmo, M. A. (2011). The anti-inflammatory effects of exercise: mechanisms and implications for the prevention and treatment of disease. Nature reviews immunology, 11(9), 607-615.

Göran Eriksson. (2022) Promoting extreme fitness regimes through the communicative affordances of reality makeover television: a multimodal critical discourse analysis. Critical Studies in Media Communication 39:5, pages 408-426.

Halliwell, E., & Harvey, M. (2006). Examination of a sociocultural model of disordered eating among male and female adolescents. British journal of health psychology, 11(2), 235-248.

Hausenblas, H. A., & Fallon, E. A. (2006). Exercise and body image: A meta-analysis. Psychology and health, 21(1), 33-47.

Heilig, M., Epstein, D. H., Nader, M. A., & Shaham, Y. (2016). Time to connect: bringing social context into addiction neuroscience. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 17(9), 592–599. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.67

Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., & Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on psychological science, 10(2), 227-237.

Hotard, S. R., McFatter, R. M., McWhirter, R. M., & Stegall, M. E. (1989). Interactive effects of extraversion, neuroticism, and social relationships on subjective well-being. Journal of personality and social psychology, 57(2), 321.

Ignatius, E., & Kokkonen, M. (2007). Factors contributing to verbal self-disclosure. Nordic Psychology, 59(4), 362-391.

Inagaki, T. K., Hazlett, L. I., & Andreescu, C. (2020). Opioids and social bonding: Effect of naltrexone on feelings of social connection and ventral striatum activity to close others. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(4), 732.

Johnson, M. B., & Thiese, S. M. (1992). A review of overtraining syndrome—recognizing the signs and symptoms. Journal of athletic training, 27(4), 352.

Laing, R. D. (1994). The divided self. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 165(3), 420-423.

Lewis, R. G., Florio, E., Punzo, D., & Borrelli, E. (2021). The Brain’s reward system in health and disease. Circadian Clock in Brain Health and Disease, 57-69.

Mellor, D., Stokes, M., Firth, L., Hayashi, Y., & Cummins, R. (2008). Need for belonging, relationship satisfaction, loneliness, and life satisfaction. Personality and individual differences, 45(3), 213-218.

Pantell, M., Rehkopf, D., Jutte, D., Syme, S. L., Balmes, J., & Adler, N. (2013). Social isolation: a predictor of mortality comparable to traditional clinical risk factors. American journal of public health, 103(11), 2056-2062.

Rachel Wurzman How isolation fuels opioid addiction Posted Oct 2018

Ricciardelli, L. A., McCabe, M. P., Holt, K. E., & Finemore, J. (2003). A biopsychosocial model for understanding body image and body change strategies among children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24(4), 475-495.

Rook, K. S. (1987b). Social support versus companionship: Effects on life stress, loneliness, and evaluations by others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52. 1132-1147.

Schorb, A., Niebauer, J., Aichhorn, W., Schiepek, G., Scherr, J., & Claussen, M. C. (2021). Overtraining from a sports psychiatry perspective. Deutsche Zeitschrift für Sportmedizin, 72(6), 271-279.

Seigel, K., & Hetta, J. (2001). Exercise and eating disorder symptoms among young females. Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 6, 32-39.

Seifert, T. (2005). Anthropomorphic characteristics of centerfold models: Trends towards slender figures over time. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 37(3), 271-274.

Smith, L. L. (2000). Cytokine hypothesis of overtraining: a physiological adaptation to excessive stress?. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 32(2), 317.

Thompson, R. A., & Trattner Sherman, R. (1999). Athletes, athletic performance, and eating disorders: Healthier alternatives. Journal of Social Issues, 55(2), 317-337.

White, P., Young, K., & Gillett, J. (1995). Bodywork as a moral imperative: Some critical notes on health and fitness. Loisir et societe/Society and Leisure, 18(1), 159-181.

White, J., & Halliwell, E. (2010). Examination of a sociocultural model of excessive exercise among male and female adolescents. Body Image, 7(3), 227-233.

This website uses cookies to provide smooth visitor experience. Privacy Policy